Social Icons

15 January 2017

Emotions that Facilitate Language Learning: The Positive Psychology-Broadening Power of the Imagination

Published By Melissa (1507042035)

In today’s world, society is facing extremely tough challenges in the form of global warming, natural disasters, economic recession, unprecedented homelessness, terrorism and the draining continuation of war. With all this sadness and horror, where in the world does a science based on testing happiness, wellbeing, personal growth and ‘the good life’ fit into the modern-day agenda?
Positive psychology is not simply the focus on positive thinking and positive emotions. It’s much more than that. Indeed, the area of positive psychology is focused on what makes individuals and communities flourish, rather than languish. Flourishing is defined as ‘a state of positive mental health; to thrive, to prosper and to fare well in endeavours free of mental illness, filled with emotional vitality and function positively in private and social realms’ (Michalec et al., 2009: 391). Indeed, existing figures show that only 18 per cent of adults meet the criteria of flourishing, 65 per cent are moderately mentally healthy and 17 per cent are languishing. Unsurprisingly, flourishing has several positive correlates such as academic achievement, mastery goal setting, higher levels of self-control and continued perseverance (Howell, 2009). Thus, a science that focuses on the development and facilitation of flourishing environments and individuals is an important addition to the psychological sciences.
Positive psychology concentrates on positive experiences at three time points: (1) the past, centering on wellbeing, contentment and satisfaction; (2) the present, which focuses on concepts such as happiness and flow experiences; (3) the future, with concepts including optimism and hope. Not only does positive psychology distinguish between wellbeing across time points but it also separates the subject area into three nodes:
■ the subjective node, which encompasses things like positive experiences and states across past, present and future (for example, happiness, optimism, wellbeing);
■ the individual node, which focuses on characteristics of the ‘good person’ (for example, talent, wisdom, love, courage, creativity); and
■ the group node, which studies positive institutions, citizenship and communities (for example, altruism, tolerance, work ethic) (Positive Psychology Center, 1998).
Contrary to criticism, positive psychology is not a selfish psychology. At its best, positive psychology has been able to give the scientific community, society and individuals a new perspective on existing ideas as well as providing empirical evidence to support the phenomenon of human flourishing. Above all, though, positive psychology has challenged and rebalanced the deficit approach to living while connecting its findings to many different disciplines. Throughout this textbook you will see how inducing positive emotions, committing acts of kindness and enhancing social connections enable individual and societal flourishing, demonstrating the usefulness of the discipline for individual, group and community wellbeing.




Applications of Positive Psychology
Positive psychology can have a range of real-world applications in areas including education, therapy, self-help, stress management, and workplace issues. Using strategies from positive psychology, teachers, coaches, therapists, and employers can motivate others and help individuals understand and develop their personal strengths. 

The Goals of Positive Psychology

The goals of Positive Psychology mirror the goals of language learning.  Most professionals enter the field of teaching in order to make a difference in the lives of students.  What motivates teachers to continue is the reinforcement of seeing students thrive and perform at optimal levels.  Positive Psychology seeks to do the same, promoting general well-being and life satisfaction across the broader spectrum of individuals and institutions.
Positive psychology is a rapidly expanding subfield in psychology that has important implications for the field of second language acquisition (SLA). Positive Psychology also has application in all aspects of teaching and learning, from pre-school level to post graduate, for faculty and students alike. The concept of positive education seeks both higher academic achievement, increased character strengths, self awareness and emotional control, self efficacy (not self-esteem), resilience, flexible and accurate thinking skills, strategies for positive relationships and learned optimism. There are reasons for both encouragement and caution as studies inspired by positive psychology are undertaken.

POSITIVE PSYCHOLOGY AND LANGUAGE LEARNING
On the trail of positive psychology in SLA As with the field of psychology, PP in SLA could perhaps be viewed as having a short history and a long past. Lake (2013) was one of the first to explicitly adapt and apply PP concepts in his study of Japanese learners’ positive self, positive L2 self, self-efficacy, and intended effort. Lake successfully demonstrated that PP-inspired measures correlate with effort, self-efficacy, and TOEIC Bridge scores. There are several additional, established concepts familiar to those studying SLA that could also be brought under a PP umbrella. Although there are a number of lines of inquiry that we could consider, let us focus on five especially salient ones: the humanistic movement in language teaching, models of motivation that draw upon a range of affective factors, the concept of an affective filter, studies of the good language learner, and the more recent literature on concepts related to the self and its development.
The humanistic movement in language teaching was at its peak in the 1970s and 1980s. Characteristically, humanistic approaches took a holistic view of the learner, combining cognition and affect; the underlying assumption, as expressed by Roberts (as cited in Stevick, 1990), being that “the affective aspects of language learning are as important as the cognitive aspects, [and therefore] the learner should be treated in some sense as a 'whole person'” (p.26). Today few would dispute such core principles and their importance for understanding language learner psychology and behaviours. However, humanism became closely associated with alternative forms of language teaching such as Suggestopedia, The Silent Way, and Total Physical Response (Asher, 1969; Gattegno, 1963; Lozanov, 1979). As with humanistic psychology, these instructional methods came under considerable criticism for their lack of scientific support and validity; however, the humanistic tradition as a guiding approach and epistemology has had a considerable, lasting influence in SLA. Indeed, integrating affect and cognition remains a key tenet of many contemporary SLA models. In particular, work by R. C. Gardner and his colleagues has played an important role in drawing attention to socio-affective factors and the importance of positive attitudes toward the language, its speakers and related cultures (Gardner & Lambert, 1972; Gardner, 1985, 2010). In the socio-educational model, Gardner (2010) holds that positive attitudes towards the learning situation (teacher and course) facilitate language learning. The social milieu in which the learning takes place is a key source of both positive and negative attitudes, as learners internalize elements of the context in which they live. Clément’s (1980, 1986) socio-contextual model places even more emphasis on the learning context, the power relationships among language groups, and tensions between language acquisition and language loss, especially for members of a minority group learning the language of a majority group. In addition, Clément proposes a secondary motivational process within the individual, self-confidence, which is defined by low anxiety and perceptions of high linguistic competence. Both Gardner’s and Clément’s models foreground the role of social and cultural contexts, highlighting that an individual’s psychology does not exist in a vacuum; an individual learner is always embedded in multiple contexts.
A third perspective of note is offered by Krashen (1985), who also drew attention to the role of emotions with what he termed the affective filter. Krashen argued that a high degree of negative emotion raises a filter that reduces the amount of comprehensible input reaching the learner. Conversely, in the presence of positive emotions, the affective filter is lower and the learner is open to being exposed to more comprehensible input. A fourth noteworthy approach, studies of the “good language learners.” (GLLs; Naiman, 1978, Rubin, 1975; Stern, 1975; Stevick, 1989), provides recognizable parallels to the holistic approach of Maslow’s studies of self-actualizing persons. Essentially, GLL studies sought to understand the lessons that can be learned from “good” or “expert” learners or teachers (Griffiths, 2008). The related set of studies of “expertise” (see, e.g., Farrell, 2013; Johnson, 2005; Tsui, 2003) also examined positive examples of learners and/or teachers, typically focussing on the integrated use of competences and skills. An important feature of many GLL studies has been the tendency to look at the processes by which “good” language learners acquire a foreign language and not just merely describe the learner and their language output. Such a process-oriented approach to understanding the learner resonates with more recent developments in complexity perspectives.
Most recently, a fifth line of relevant research has developed that emphasizes the self as a central concept. With respect to motivation, Dörnyei’s (2005) L2 self system model proposes that people are motivated to deal with perceived discrepancies between their current sense of self and their future selves, one to move closer to their vision of an ideal self and another to prevent developing negative aspects of the self, such as a sense of failure or disappointing important others. This approach stresses the importance of having positive future goals, a requisite level of optimism that one is able to change and potentially achieve these future self-states, as well as the strategic knowledge necessary in order to achieve future goals. Related studies investigating the self in its various forms  also highlight the role of positive self-beliefs, a sense of competence, a growth mindset and accompanying optimism about the potential for positive change in one’s abilities (e.g., Mercer & Ryan, 2010; Mercer & Williams, 2014).

Positive and negative emotion
One of the most important findings in the PP literature thus far has been Fredrickson’s (Fredrickson, 2001, 2003; Fredrickson & Branigan, 2005) clarification of the difference between positive and negative emotion (MacIntyre & Gregersen, 2012), that is, emotions that are generally experienced by individuals as pleasant versus unpleasant. Emotion research has been successful in identifying a relatively small number of basic, universal emotions that are closely tied to physiological responding and basic survival (emotions such as fear, disgust, anger, and so on). Fredrickson’s broaden-and-build theory (2001, 2003) examines the nature and function of positive emotion. Fredrickson concludes that positive and negative emotions are not dichotomous or opposite ends of the same continuum (as was implied by Krashen’s concept of the affective filter); they are better conceptualized as two dimensions of experience. At the core, the function of positive emotion is qualitatively different from negative emotion. Whereas negative emotion tends to narrow a person’s field of attention and predisposes specific action tendencies, positive emotion creates tendencies toward play and exploration, yielding a broadened field of attention and building resources for future action.
The implications of positive-broadening emotions for SLA can be profound. Differentiating positive and negative emotions leads to a more nuanced understanding of how they affect L2 learning and communication. The most widely studied emotion in SLA has been anxiety (Dewaele & MacIntyre, this volume). Prior research has presented a conventional, one-dimensional view of positive and negative emotion using a see-saw metaphor (positive goes up, negative goes down). A two-dimensional view of emotion can accommodate the see-saw view of emotion, but also opens up the possibility of examining ambivalence (MacIntyre, 2007) which is the co-occurrence of negative with positive emotion (see Table 2). Ambivalence is a common experience in SLA; for example, when a person is feeling both confident and anxious before giving a classroom presentation or approaching a native speaker. Further, a twodimensional view allows for understanding amotivation, apathy or the absence of emotion in the situation as the lack of arousal of both positive and negative emotions in a particular context.

Goals of language learning
For us, as with many language educators, one of the goals is to foster the positivity of our learners’ educational experiences, and supporting them as individuals in reaching their personal highest levels of achievement and success (Fredrickson, 2001). Whilst learners’ ultimate levels of achievement and proficiency will be a focus of SLA research, perhaps a vital additional perspective would be to focus on the processes and timescales in which learners can be seen to be happy and experience flourishing in language learning. Griffiths (2008, pp. 1-2) asks key questions in her collection on the GLL such as, what is it that makes a good language learner? And why are some learners more successful than others? Viewed through a PP lens, these questions might take a slightly different focus concentrating not on levels of proficiency, language competence and achievement, but instead considering the processes, rather than the product of learning. The following are example questions that capture processes over different timescales:
1. Over the long term (measured in years), why are some learners happier, more resilient, and enjoy language learning more than others?
2. In what ways/how do learners appear to develop a sense of flourishing whilst engaging in language learning (activities) during a specific course (measured in days and months)?
3. What features of short-term, immediate experience are associated with the ebb and flow of engagement in the learning situation, such as a classroom, during the minutes that teacher and students spend together? Can we better understand what leads to greater engagement for some learners and not others (measured in seconds and minutes)?
4. How do learners’ immediate experiences interact with their mediumto-long-term emotional trajectories whilst learning a foreign language (measured on multiple timescales)?
As research progresses with a process-oriented approach, positive experiences can be understood in more nuanced terms as we consider not only moment-tomoment experiences but also how ongoing experiences fit within the language and self-development process. Kahneman and Riis (2005) draw a valuable distinction between these two processes that they link to the conceptualization of self. First, the experiencing self, with a timeframe of approximately 3 seconds, is our ongoing window of conscious experience; the experiencing self is active and fleeting. Second, the remembering self has a long history in memory. The remembering self knits together the narrative that captures the meaning of events in our life, and is open to reinterpretation. Kahneman’s (2011) studies of the experience of happiness draw the distinction between the experiencing and remembering selves, helping to clarify ways in which immediate negative events can ultimately be interpreted within a positive narrative or vice versa. Thus, the multiple processes involved in learning a foreign language might produce a complex answer to a deceptively simple question such as “what makes learners happy?” When we consider these different perspectives on experiences?

Four encouraging trends for the future of PP research in SLA
If PP is to be taken up in SLA, it needs to develop along strong empirical lines. In considering the potential for PP-inspired research to have an impact on SLA, we see four trends that support emerging research in this area.
First, the social turn in SLA means that the field is taking seriously the idea that contexts in which language learning occurs are diverse, nuanced, and they matter. The three pillars of PP include positive emotions, positive character traits and positive institutions. Conducting studies of enabling institutions, so far, has been the weakest link for PP (Waterman, 2013). However, in SLA research, greater care is now being taken to describe the contexts in which learning occurs, especially at the classroom level. Perhaps the next step is to focus a little more on the institutions (broadly defined formal and informal organizational structures) that enable success and promote positive language learning environments. Institutions are embedded in the broader cultural milieu that will shape the ways in which the aims of PP are pursued and various possible configurations of its pillars (see Leu, Wang, & Koo, 2011; Sheldon, 2009).
A second trend that bodes well for developing interest in PP is the idea of complex dynamic systems, which has become a “hot” topic in SLA (MacIntyre, Dörnyei, & Henry, 2015). Larsen-Freeman and Cameron (2008) have argued that L2 development involves complex, dynamic, emergent, open, self-organizing and adaptive systems. Systems thinking adopts inherently holistic perspectives and examines dynamism across different contexts and timescales. Models of the learning and communication process are incomplete without explicit consideration of positive emotions, individual strengths, and the various institutions and contexts of learning, such as governments, public/private schools, community groups, and networks in which learning occurs. In addition, Lazarus (2003) questioned whether one can consider any emotion positive or negative. In a functional sense, all emotions are adaptive (e.g., fear serves a protective function). Quickly moving between positive and negative affective states, and explicitly considering ambivalent states (MacIntyre, 2007), is one of the strengths that dynamic models have over prior approaches to individual differences research (MacIntyre & Serroul, 2015).
A third reason that PP topics might be especially relevant to SLA is the methodological diversity already present in the field. As noted previously, core epistemological advances have been made toward reconciling the humanistic and PP traditions that sometimes seem impossible in psychology (Waterman, 2013). However, SLA research has been receptive to a variety of quantitative, qualitative, and mixed methods research for several years now, with studies routinely collecting data from multiple types of sources. The strength of large- scale quantitative surveys lies in assessing the reliability and generalizability of the findings, but the weakness is that general trends in large datasets might not apply to any single individual within those data sets. On the other hand, the strength of individual-level qualitative data is that a rich description of the relevant factors for an individual can be proffered, with the weakness that reliability and generalizability typically cannot be assessed. The strength of each method is the weakness of the other (Creswell, 2003). It is highly desirable to maintain the empirical base for PP in SLA, and the diversity of rigorous research approaches already available is encouraging.

A fourth, related sign that SLA is ready for PP is what might be called an “individual turn” (MacIntyre, 2014). The dense data that is required to study in detail the processes of language learning often must be collected and interpreted at the individual level (de Bot, Lowie, & Verspoor, 2007). Research that uses group averages and correlations among variables has been a mainstay of SLA theory development over the years (Dörnyei & Taguchi, 2010). More recently, alongside the existing large-sample methods, there has been a burgeoning interest in documenting the complexity of individual cases; it is for this reason we have opened this paper with the personal stories of Csikszentmihalyi and Seligman. Individual level research can describe in some detail the processes that lead to happiness, the protective force of learned optimism, or describe the most enjoyable facets of learning for a specific person, with nomothetic studies identifying how commonly occurring these events might be. An example of a dense, individual-level, mixed methods approach (quantitative and qualitative) to SLA research can be found in MacIntyre and Serroul’s (2015) idio dynamic study that integrates motivation with perceived competence, anxiety and willingness to communicate as part of an ongoing, dynamic process. Perhaps we are at an optimal time for a wide ranging research program devoted to study the role of PP in SLA from both the individual and group perspectives.

References:

Baumgartner, H., Pieters, R., & Bagozzi, R. (2008). Future-oriented emotions: Conceptualization and behavioral effects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(4), 685-696. Retrieved from https://repozytorium.amu.edu.pl/jspui/bitstream/
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (2011). Positive psychology: Where did it come from, where is it going? In M. K. Sheldon, T. B. Kashdan, & M. F. Steger (Eds), Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward (pp.3-8). New York: Oxford University Press. Retrieved from https://repozytorium.amu.edu.pl/
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218-226.
Gable, S. & Haidt, J (2005). What (and Why) is Positive Psychology? Review of General Psychology, 9(2), 103–110. Retrieved from http://psychology.about.com/
Lake, J. (2013). Positive L2 self: Linking positive psychology with L2 motivation. In M. Apple, D. Da Silva, & T Fellner (Eds.), Language learning motivation in Japan (pp 225-244). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. Retrieved from https://repozytorium.amu.edu.pl/jspui/bitstream/
Seligman, M. E. P. & Csikszenmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55, 5-14. Retrieved from

No comments:

Post a Comment

 
 
Blogger Templates